Photovoice

After watching, what are your thoughts? Did this video help clarify what is meant by intersectionality for you (if so, what became clearer?)? What would you change to make the analogy fit better to your experiences? (3-4 sentences will do, more always welcome!)

Then, please read Cole, E. R. (2009). Intersectionality and research in psychology. American

Psychologist, 64(3), 170-180. I will put the PDF of this article in the Announcements section. Please answer the following questions:

Whose concerns and what concerns brought about the birth of the

concept of intersectionality?

What did Crenshaw critique in law and legal studies?

What are the 3 major questions Cole recommends?

 For each of the 3 questions raised by Cole-

 What is the purpose of this question?

 Provide an example of bias in research this question aims to

address?

 How does the question help improve research?

 What are the benefits of this question? How will psychological

science get better?

As always, please avoid direct quotations from the article- I’m interested in your thoughts!

Intersectionality and Research in Psychology

Elizabeth R. Cole University of Michigan

Feminist and critical race theories offer the concept of intersectionality to describe analytic approaches that si- multaneously consider the meaning and consequences of multiple categories of identity, difference, and disadvan- tage. To understand how these categories depend on one another for meaning and are jointly associated with out- comes, reconceptualization of the meaning and signifi- cance of the categories is necessary. To accomplish this, the author presents 3 questions for psychologists to ask: Who is included within this category? What role does inequality play? Where are there similarities? The 1st question involves attending to diversity within social cate- gories. The 2nd conceptualizes social categories as con- noting hierarchies of privilege and power that structure social and material life. The 3rd looks for commonalities across categories commonly viewed as deeply different. The author concludes with a discussion of the implications and value of these 3 questions for each stage of the re- search process.

Keywords: intersectionality, feminist psychology, race, theory

Psychologists are increasingly concerned with theeffects of race/ethnicity, gender, social class, andsexuality on outcomes such as health and well- being, personal and social identities, and political views and participation. However, little work has considered how these categories of identity, difference, and disadvantage are jointly associated with outcomes. Silverstein (2006) found that among the publications dealing with either gen- der or race indexed in PsycINFO between 2002 and 2004, only a minority investigated both constructs, perhaps be- cause psychologists generally aim to simplify models for parsimony, either by omitting variables or by statistically controlling for membership in categories other than the one of interest (Betancourt & López, 1993). Even less attention has been paid to how social categories depend on one another for meaning, despite the obvious fact that every individual necessarily occupies multiple categories (i.e., gender, race, class, etc.) simultaneously.

Such questions may be understood within the rubric of intersectionality, which feminist and critical race theorists developed to describe analytic approaches that consider the meaning and consequences of multiple categories of social group membership. However, psychologists have been slow to incorporate this concept into their work because there are no established guidelines for empirically address- ing research questions informed by an intersectional frame- work (McCall, 2005). Given this gap, some psychologists

might imagine that to address intersectional questions, it is necessary to develop complex designs involving prohibi- tively large samples or to enlist the cooperation of an interdisciplinary team to triangulate the problem. Although this is not the case, an intersectionality framework does ask researchers to examine categories of identity, difference, and disadvantage with a new lens. This article aims to explicate this framework for psychologists by describing the theoretical rationale underlying intersectionality and outlining a series of three questions psychologists can ask to conceptualize the influences of multiple social catego- ries. I conclude by discussing the implications and value of these questions for the research process.

History of the Concept of Intersectionality

The Combahee River Collective (1977/1995), a group of Black feminists, wrote a manifesto that has been cited as one of the earliest expressions of intersectionality (see also Beale, 1970). They argued “We . . . find it difficult to separate race from class from sex oppression because in our lives they are most often experienced simultaneously” (Combahee River Collective, 1977/1995, p. 234). In fact, the concept has deeper roots. In the United States, Black scholar–activists have long theorized this position and at- tempted to incorporate it into their politics. Late in the 19th century, Anna Julia Cooper exhorted Black male leaders to include sexist discrimination faced by Black women in their race-based agenda (Giddings, 1985). Not long after- ward, W. E. B. DuBois challenged the U.S. communist party to incorporate an analysis of race into their class- based organizing (Hancock, 2005). Despite these early framings, King (1988) showed that major U.S. social movements organized on the basis of race, class, and gen- der failed to consider the intersections of these categories in their political analysis and organizing. Consequently, the interests of those who experienced multiple forms of sub-

Elizabeth R. Cole, Department of Women’s Studies, Department of Psy- chology, and Center for Afroamerican and African Studies, University of Michigan.

A version of this article was presented as the Yulee Endowed Lecture at George Washington University in Washington, DC, on February 27, 2006. I thank Ramaswami Mahalingam, Peggy McCracken, Yopie Prins, Abigail J. Stewart, Elizabeth Wingrove, and Alyssa N. Zucker for their helpful feedback on the article. Any errors are mine alone.

Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Eliz- abeth R. Cole, University of Michigan, Department of Women’s Studies, 204 South State Street, Ann Arbor, MI 48109-1290. E-mail: ecole@ umich.edu

170 April 2009 ● American Psychologist © 2009 American Psychological Association 0003-066X/09/$12.00

Vol. 64, No. 3, 170–180 DOI: 10.1037/a0014564

ordination (e.g., Black women, working-class Blacks) were often poorly served (see also Purdie-Vaughns & Eibach, 2008).

The early 1980s saw an upsurge in scholarship about race and gender by women of color (e.g., Anzaldua, 1987; Davis, 1983; Giddings, 1985; hooks, 1984; Hull, Scott, & Smith, 1982). Without naming the theory driving their investigations, this work addressed intersections of race, gender, and often class and sexuality. Legal scholar and critical race theorist Kimberle Crenshaw (1989/1993) is credited with originating the term intersectionality, but almost concurrently other scholars were drawing attention to the limitations of analyses isolating race or gender as the primary category of identity, difference, or disadvantage (Hancock, 2007a; e.g., Collins, 1990; Hurtado, 1989; Smith & Stewart, 1983). In a groundbreaking work, Cren- shaw critiqued the “single-axis framework that is dominant in antidiscrimination law . . . feminist theory and anti-racist politics” (p. 383) for its focus on the experiences of the most privileged members of subordinate groups. She ar- gued that legal cases revealed Black women plaintiffs

sometimes experience discrimination in ways similar to white women’s experiences; sometimes they share very similar experi- ences with Black men. Yet often they experience double discrim- ination—the combined effects of practices which discriminate on the basis of race, and on the basis of sex. And sometimes, they experience discrimination as Black women—not the sum of race and sex discrimination, but as Black women. (Crenshaw, 1989/ 1993, p. 385)

Note that she described three permutations: similar experiences, additive or multiplicative effects (double dis- crimination or double jeopardy), and experiences specific to their status as Black women. Although scholars today often distinguish between the additive or multiple approach

and intersectionality (Hancock, 2007a; Stewart & McDer- mott, 2004), each of the permutations Crenshaw offered are viable hypotheses about how multiple social statuses might be experienced simultaneously and might be included un- der the rubric of intersectional analyses.

Each of these early articulations of intersectionality focused on the experiences of groups holding multiple disadvantaged statuses; in doing so, they highlighted the ways that analyses considering categories such as race and gender independently may be limited because, in practice, individuals experience these statuses simultaneously. How- ever, a corollary to this observation is that some members of disadvantaged groups also hold privileged identities (e.g., middle-class Blacks, White women). This reveals that although much of the literature on intersectionality has been theorized from the standpoint of those who experience multiple dimensions of disadvantage, this framework can also inform how privileged groups are understood.

The concept of intersectionality is a signal contribu- tion of feminist studies (McCall, 2005; Risman, 2004); in some academic circles, the phrase race–class–gender is invoked so frequently that it has been called a mantra (Fine & Burns, 2003). However, too often this triad is mentioned without meaningfully addressing the concerns for which the phrase serves as shorthand (Knapp, 2005). This may be inevitable until psychologists develop new ways to use the theory of intersectionality to conceptualize how social cat- egories jointly shape experiences and outcomes.

Intersectional Conceptualizations of Social Categories: Three Questions Toward this end, I propose three questions psychologists might ask as a strategy for addressing intersectional ques- tions in psychology research: First, who is included within this category? Second, what role does inequality play? Third, where are there similarities? The first question in- volves attending to diversity within social categories to interrogate how the categories depend on one other for meaning. The second question conceptualizes social cate- gories as connoting hierarchies of privilege and power that structure social and material life. The third question looks for commonalities cutting across categories often viewed as deeply different. These questions are not mutually exclu- sive; in fact, each question builds on insights generated by the previous one.

To demonstrate the fruitfulness of asking these ques- tions, I draw on examples from research in psychology and related social sciences on aspects of women’s sexuality, including ideals of feminine appearance, sexual respect- ability, and risk in intimate relationships. Conceptualizing categories of identity, difference, and disadvantage in terms of these questions has implications for each stage of the research process, which I discuss later (see Table 1).

1. Who Is Included Within This Category?

At the simplest level, psychologists can begin to consider the intersectional nature of the social categories they study by reflecting on who is included within a category. This

Elizabeth R. Cole Photo by Scott C. Soderberg

171April 2009 ● American Psychologist

question draws researchers’ attention to diversity within categories. Because certain groups have been systemati- cally underrepresented in psychology research (e.g., people of color, S. Sue, 1999; poor women, Reid, 1993), subcat- egories that only partially represent a larger category have often been taken as representative of the whole category. For example, because of the use of student samples (S. Sue, 1999), much of what is known about women in psychology is based on responses from women who are White and often middle class. An intersectional approach is an anti- dote to this erasure.

Moreover, the question may also encourage research- ers to study groups belonging to multiple subordinated categories, such as women from racial/ethnic minority groups. This attention to those who have traditionally been excluded, perhaps the oldest approach within intersection- ality studies, thwarts any tendency to view a category in essentialist terms, both by illuminating what is overlooked when a social category is assumed to include only certain (usually privileged) subgroups of that category and by representing diverse experiences contained within catego- ries defined by multiple identities (e.g., the category of Black women includes women of different social classes and sexualities). Asking who is included within a category can facilitate representation of those who have been over- looked and the repair of misconceptions in the extant literature. The need for representation was well illustrated by early work on intersectionality showing that a single- axis framework that defines disadvantage only in terms of

group members who are otherwise privileged systemati- cally excludes members of multiply subordinated groups (Crenshaw, 1989/1993; King, 1988).

However, turning scholarly attention to groups who experience disadvantage based on membership in multiple categories is more than a matter of equity or inclusiveness. Such inclusion transcends representation, offering the pos- sibility to repair misconceptions engendered by the erasure of minority groups and the marginal subgroups within them. First, by focusing on groups that have been ne- glected, researchers are better able to arrive at a contextu- alized understanding of the groups’ experiences, rather than viewing them in terms of the way they depart from norms based on dominant groups (Weber & Parra-Medina, 2003). Second, analyses that presume to focus on, say, gender, without consideration of other category member- ships, implicitly assume a host of other social statuses that usually go unnamed in American culture: middle-class standing, heterosexuality, able-bodiedness, and White race (D. W. Sue, 2004). Scholars who attend to which groups are represented and which tend to be excluded—either by focusing their work on members of subordinate groups (hooks, 1984) or, conversely, by explicitly identifying and investigating the multiple identities that define privilege (see, e.g., Farough, 2006; Kuriloff & Reichert, 2003)— disrupt these assumptions by identifying the ways that race, class, or other identities shape the meaning of gender (Higginbotham, 1992).

Table 1 Implications of the Three Questions for Each Stage of the Research Process

Research stage

Question

Who is included within this category? What role does inequality play? Where are the similarities?

Generation of hypotheses

Is attuned to diversity within categories

Literature review attends to social and historical contexts of inequality

May be exploratory rather than hypothesis testing to discover similarities

Sampling Focuses on neglected groups Category memberships mark groups with unequal access to power and resources

Includes diverse groups connected by common relationships to social and institutional power

Operationalization Develops measures from the perspective of the group being studied

If comparative, differences are conceptualized as stemming from structural inequality (upstream) rather than as primarily individual-level differences

Views social categories in terms of individual and institutional practices rather than primarily as characteristics of individuals

Analysis Attends to diversity within a group and may be conducted separately for each group studied

Tests for both similarities and differences

Interest is not limited to differences

Interpretation of findings

No group’s findings are interpreted to represent a universal or normative experience

Differences are interpreted in light of groups’ structural positions

Sensitivity to nuanced variations across groups is maintained even when similarities are identified

172 April 2009 ● American Psychologist

Such attention is critical because failure to attend to how social categories depend on one another for meaning renders knowledge of any one category both incomplete and biased. This was illustrated by Spelman’s (1988) cri- tique of Chodorow’s claims about the universal character- istics of mothering. Spelman noted that Chodorow consid- ered only the specific practices of Western families with race and class privilege; Spelman concluded that “it is theoretically significant . . . if statements that appear to be true about ‘men and women’ clearly aren’t true when we specify that we are talking about men and women of different classes or races” (p. 80). Similarly, Savin-Wil- liams and Diamond (2000) showed that general models of the development of same-sex erotic identity were based primarily on men’s experience and did not reflect the trajectories of lesbians and bisexual women. In this case, attempting to conceptualize sexual minority as a status or identity that exists independently of gender has little meaning.

Research investigating perceptions of the body among women of color illustrates how attending to diversity within groups can productively complicate the understand- ing of a category such as women. Using survey methods, Schooler, Ward, Merriwether, and Caruthers (2004) found that the more hours White women spent viewing television shows featuring mainly White casts, the greater their body dissatisfaction. Watching the same mainstream programs had no effect on Black women’s body satisfaction, and Black women who watched programs with predominantly Black casts reported higher satisfaction. The authors ar- gued that observing media images involves different pro- cesses for White and Black women. White women may view the thin and perfectly groomed White actresses as competitors representing an unattainable ideal; Black women, aware of the stigma on their group, may view attractive Black actresses as allies and role models. For this sample, ethnic identity was also a positive predictor of Black women’s body satisfaction. In contrast, Lau, Lum, Chronister, and Forrest (2006) found that Asian American women experienced a kind of double bind. Those who reported that media influenced their appearance ideals had greater body dissatisfaction, but the same was true of women reporting greater endorsement of Asian values (e.g., collectivism, adherence to family norms). Unlike the Black women studied by Schooler et al., psychological engagement with the minority culture was not associated with satisfaction among Asian American women.

These examples suggest several ways that attending to who is included within a category can lead to a more nuanced understanding of how social categories of identity, difference, and disadvantage shape experience. First, con- sidering groups that have traditionally been overlooked may lead researchers to hypothesize about different predic- tors. Because both studies were sensitive to the ways that women’s experiences of social norms for feminine appear- ance are shaped by race/ethnicity, they included variables, such as cultural values and ethnic identity, as predictors for body dissatisfaction. Second, as researchers increase their understanding of how one social category is shaped by another—in this case, how gender is shaped by race/eth-

nicity—they can begin to reread silences in the extant literature as well. Schooler et al.’s (2004) work encourages researchers to construe much of what psychologists have already learned about women’s body image (e.g., media representations are harmful) to be knowledge about White women’s body image. Third, knowing more about diversity within a category may help psychologists envision more ways of creating treatment interventions and social change to benefit all members of the category. For example, Schooler et al. concluded that Black women’s experiences suggest that under certain circumstances, media images could have a beneficial impact on viewers.

Considering who is included within a category accom- plishes more than mere inclusion; it improves psycholo- gists’ ability to theorize and empirically investigate the ways social categories structure individual and social life across the board. Thus, intersectionality is not only a tool to understand the experiences of minority group members. Nevertheless, increasing attention to diversity within social groups is not sufficient to address the psychological mean- ing of race, gender, and other social categories. Sociolo- gists remind researchers that the social practices that con- struct race and gender involve hierarchy and inequality (Bonilla-Silva, 1997; Risman, 2004). Yet, when research- ers attend to who is included within the social categories they study, with particular attention to groups that have been traditionally overlooked, social and material inequal- ity between groups may be treated only implicitly (why, after all, have some groups been studied to the exclusion of others?). These concerns are addressed by the second question.

2. What Role Does Inequality Play? Categories such as race, gender, social class, and sexuality do not simply describe groups that may be different or similar; they encapsulate historical and continuing relations of political, material, and social inequality and stigma. Mahalingam (2007) characterized intersectionality in terms of the “interplay between person and social location, with particular emphasis on power relations among various so- cial locations” (p. 45). Asking what role inequality plays draws attention to the ways that multiple category mem- berships position individuals and groups in asymmetrical relation to one another, affecting their perceptions, experi- ences, and outcomes. This question helps psychologists to view constructs such as race and gender as structural cat- egories and social processes rather than primarily as char- acteristics of individuals, a move consistent with recent methodological critiques (Helms, Jernigan, & Mascher, 2005) and social constructionist approaches within psy- chology (e.g., Jost & Kruglanski, 2002). Moreover, soci- ologists argue that constructs like race (Bonilla-Silva, 1997) and gender (Risman, 2004) affect beliefs about what is possible or desirable and define the contours of individ- uals’ opportunities and life chances through social and institutional practices. Considering the role of inequality helps psychologists see individuals as embedded in cultural and historical contexts, a tradition that has deep roots within the discipline but one that has languished recently.

173April 2009 ● American Psychologist

Hurtado (1989) theorized how structural inequality shapes contact between women who differ by race/ethnic- ity, arguing that the interests of White women and women of color are deeply divided by their relationship to White men, the most privileged race/gender group in American culture. As wives, mothers, and daughters of White men, White women derive social and economic benefits from existing inequities; thus, even those who are feminists may participate in a form of complicity with the status quo. In contrast, women of color generally have no vested interest in placating White men in personal relationships and, thus, have more latitude in their consciousness, resistance, and protest. This analysis led Hurtado to argue that “the defi- nition of woman is constructed differently for White women and women of color, though gender is the marking mechanism through which the subordination of each is maintained” (p. 845). These dynamics make the divisions within feminism between White women and women of color intelligible and predictable.

Femininity, long conceptualized within psychology in terms of traits and/or behavior, provides a rich test case for such an analysis. Girls and women are pressured to con- form to feminine norms, including beauty, cultivation of feminine traits, performance of normative heterosexuality including motherhood, development of domestic skills, and sexual restraint. For much of U.S. history, however, eco- nomic exploitation, stereotyping, and lack of legal protec- tion (Collins, 1990) served to deny Black women (and other women of color; see, e.g., Espiritu, 2001) the protec- tions femininity is purported to afford. This history led Collins (2004) to argue that these benchmarks of femininity “become a normative yardstick for all femininities in which Black women [and other women of color] are relegated to the bottom of the gender hierarchy” (p. 193; Higginbotham, 1992). In response, Black women activists have long as- serted their femininity, and accordingly their respectability, as a means to claim entitlement to legal protection and civil rights (Giddings, 1985).

Cole and Zucker (2007) explored Black and White women’s perceptions of femininity in light of this history. Confirmatory factor analysis of national survey data showed both groups used the same dimensions to concep- tualize femininity: feminine traits, appearance, and tradi- tional gender beliefs. However, for White women, tradi- tional gender ideology was negatively related to feminist identification. Among Black women, those who placed a high value on wearing feminine clothing were more likely to identify as feminist, and Black women rated appearance items as more important to them. Black women were also more likely than White women to identify as feminists, arguably because the experience of racial oppression sen- sitizes Black women to issues of sexism. Craig’s (2002) historical research can help explain why these aspects of femininity have different political meaning for Black and White women: Black women have traditionally used a strategy of scrupulous attention to appearance to challenge stereotypes of Blacks as uncivilized and sexually immoral. Thus, Black and White women’s social locations, defined by structural relations of inequality rooted in history and

culture, explained patterns of similarity and difference in the findings: Black and White women had similar views about the components of normative femininity; Black women reported higher levels of feminist identification because of double discrimination; and structural relations between White and Black women explain why feminine appearance bears a different association with feminism for each group. These findings address all three permutations of intersectionality as theorized by Crenshaw (1989/1993).

Similarly, Mahalingam and Leu (2005) investigated how the implicit racialization of femininity can affect members of groups who are not privileged by these ideals. They studied the experiences of Indian immigrants to the United States who work as programmers and Filipina “mail order brides.” Interviews and archival content analysis re- vealed that women from both groups asserted traditional views of femininity either to claim entitlement to the pro- tections that follow from traditional femininity or to re- deem Asian masculinity, which has often been denigrated in Western representations. Both groups contrasted their values and behavior with those of White women, whom they viewed as sexually promiscuous, lacking family ori- entation, and corrupted by feminism. They created ideal- ized gendered immigrant identities as a reaction to, and a defense against, the denigration and subordination they experienced in the United States. This research revealed similarities across these groups of Asian immigrants who face different cultural and economic circumstances. The source of this similarity lies not in a pan-Asian identity, but in the two groups’ common structural experience of racial discrimination in the United States. Thus, these Asian American groups can also be understood as similar to the African American women surveyed by Cole and Zucker (2007) in that all three groups strategically embraced pre- vailing norms of femininity in an effort to resist racial denigration. This is perhaps a surprising observation, given the widespread notion that Asian Americans represent a model minority defined by negative comparisons to African Americans and Latinos; it is the theorization of both groups as subordinated by privileged identities (albeit in different ways) that reveals this commonality.

Weber and Parra-Medina (2003) have made a useful distinction between looking “downstream” for causes (i.e., in individual behavior that might be associated with social category membership) and “upstream” at “the group pro- cesses that define systems of social inequality” (p. 190), such as laws, institutional practices, and public policies. Consideration of the role of inequality can help psycholo- gists look upstream by drawing attention to how groups stand in relation to each other and to public and private institutions, including families, schools, workplaces, and the law, and, correspondingly, how political, material, and social inequality lead to class, race, and gender differences in outcomes (see, e.g., Eagly & Wood, 1999; Glick et al., 2004; Lott, 2002; Reid, 1993). Asking this second question helps avoid the risk of treating socially constructed cate- gories as though they refer to static and ahistorical con- structs. However, to deeply engage this question, psychol- ogists would be well served to supplement their training

174 April 2009 ● American Psychologist

with interdisciplinary study in history, sociology, or other social sciences and/or to pursue collaborative relationships with scholars in other disciplines.

Just as research that considers who is included within a social category can expose the experiences of the most disadvantaged members within disadvantaged subgroups to augment psychologists’ vision of the meaning of social categories, asking what role inequality plays makes visible some ways these categories are constructed through histor- ical and ongoing social practices. Just as attention to ne- glected groups can reveal that which has been obscured in conventional analyses (e.g., the significance of race for Whites or of gender for men), intersectional analyses that conceptualize category membership in terms of inequality can help psychologists understand the relationships among groups—and group members—in societies organized around hierarchies of race, gender, sexuality, and class. Such an analysis promises that rather than merely calling for attention to the ways that these categories of identity, difference, and disadvantage intersect (Knapp, 2005), re- searchers can identify mechanisms through which they do so.

3. Where Are There Similarities? The third way to reconceptualize social categories to ad- dress intersectional research questions entails seeking sites of commonality across difference. Asking where there are similarities encourages researchers to reassess any pre- sumption that categories of identity, difference, and dis- advantage define homogeneous groups as they look for similarities that cut across categories. Looking for com- monality across difference entails viewing social cate- gories as reflecting what individuals, institutions, and cultures do, rather than simply as characteristics of indi- viduals. This shift opens up the possibility to recognize common ground between groups, even those deemed fun- damentally different by conventional categories.

This way of approaching intersectional research ques- tions is grounded in the work of authors who have used the concept as a tool for political organizing. Urging intersec- tional analysis to address important differences within groups, Crenshaw (1994) criticized agencies serving women who had experienced intimate partner violence for overlooking how statuses such as poverty and immigration status fundamentally shape certain women’s specific needs; if these needs were not addressed, the agencies were not meeting the needs of some women. Unfortunately, this key insight of intersectionality—the heterogeneity of groups— is easily misconstrued to suggest that identity groups can effectively organize around only the most specific, and thus the most limited, constituencies. Cohen (1997) exploded this misreading, advocating that social change organiza- tions should not mobilize on the basis of shared identities (which inevitably exclude some people). Instead, she noted that oppression operates through a series of interlocking systems that cut across conventional identity categories. Specifically, she suggested that lesbian and gay political activists have a limited constituency if their organizing is based only on identity. However, many of the political

issues that concern activists offer opportunities to build coalitions among diverse groups who are disadvantaged by public policies that attempt to regulate sexuality or that confer resources and privileges on the basis of sexual behavior. When seen through this lens, women on welfare targeted by marriage incentive policies have important shared interests with gay men and lesbians whose sexuality and intimate partnerships are also stigmatized and pro- scribed (Cohen, 1997).

Cohen’s (1997) argument is groundbreaking because psychologists tend to see certain identities as totalizing and determinative, as trumping all others. For example, Hig- ginbotham (1992) argued,

Race not only tends to subsume other sets of social relations, namely, gender and class, but it blurs and disguises, suppresses and negates its own complex interplay with the very social rela- tions it envelops. It precludes unity with the same gender group, but often appears to solidify people of opposing economic classes [italics added]. (p. 255)

Asch (1984) made a similar argument concerning disability. Such perceptions on the part of both laypeople and researchers can obscure both intragroup difference and loci of possible commonalities across groups.

Such insights can be powerful in research related to social issues and public policy, as these examples show. Fine and Weis (1998) interviewed poor and working-class adults about violence. Although nearly all agreed that vi- olence was a serious problem in their communities, differ- ences in the types of violence they emphasized emerged, patterned by gender and race/ethnicity. White men dis- cussed street violence by men of color, a perspective the authors likened to that of policymakers. Men of color and African American women stressed state violence, such as police brutality. White women and women of color dis- cussed domestic violence. Fine and Weis also found the groups varied in their views of the police. Many White men knew and trusted police officers, as did White women, but to a lesser extent. In contrast, the respondents of color expressed mistrust, with African American men and Lati- nas, in particular, citing police corruption. Their analysis revealed commonalities and differences that cut across simple groupings like gender or even women of color. Such a nuanced understanding would be invaluable for those planning community interventions or political organizing around this issue.

Dworkin’s (2005) work provides another example. She observed that much of the scholarly discussion of heterosexual transmission of HIV depicts women as vul- nerable and at risk from men. These representations are grounded partly in the biology of transmission, but also in gendered assumptions that women are sexually oppressed, responsive, and passive, whereas men are sexually invul- nerable, dominating, and agentic. Dworkin argued that the women-at-risk framing overlooks the ways that men vary in power and patriarchal privilege: Some heterosexual men experience sexual assault, engage in sex work, or are oth- erwise at risk because of inequities associated with race and class. This analysis underscores that it is not identity cat-

175April 2009 ● American Psychologist

egories that put individuals at risk of HIV infection but behavior and experiences. Although Dworkin made her argument discursively, she laid the groundwork for social scientists to generate hypotheses in which risk is operation- alized in terms of behavior rather than social categoriza- tion. This is not to say that categories are irrelevant: Dwor- kin also pointed to the role of power, noting that “[HIV] transmission and infection . . . [are] linked to social and economic relations of inequality” (p. 618). It is important to note, however, that she did not treat category membership as primarily an individual-level characteristic. From this perspective, similarities between heterosexual women and men who have sex with men come into view under the umbrella concept of risk. These similarities could be fertile sites of intervention or mobilizing to lobby for prevention and treatment resources.

Although grounded in insights from political organiz- ing, looking for commonality across difference suggests how an intersectional analysis can generate innovative re- search questions. The activists who developed coalition- building strategies recognized that the diversity within a group (e.g., the racial diversity among women or the class diversity among Blacks) provides opportunities to reach across perceived boundaries to identify common ground with other communities. Dworkin’s (2005) work makes clear how failing to see these commonalities raises the likelihood that researchers may misunderstand how multi- ple social structures—gender, race, sexuality—shape sex- ual behavior with potentially tragic consequences. In this, she implicitly made an argument about gender that is analogous to Helms, Jernigan, and Mascher’s (2005) re- thinking of psychologists’ methodologies for studying race; they recommended that psychologists move away from viewing race as an independent variable and instead operationalize specific mechanisms through conceptual variables. The examples I have described suggest that some research related to social issues, public policy, and practice engages these principles of coalition in an untheorized way. The concept of intersectionality offers a way to bring this insight to bear in future research.

Implications for Research To translate the theoretical insights of intersectionality into psychological research does not require the adoption of a new set of methods; rather, it requires a reconceptualization of the meaning and consequences of social categories. The extant literature on intersectionality, developed by feminist and critical race theorists, suggests three questions that can guide psychologists wishing to use this type of reconcep- tualization in their research: Who is included within this category? What role does inequality play? Where are there similarities? In this order, each question takes psycholo- gists further away from an approach in which social cate- gories are operationalized through demographic items whose meaning is self-evident; in this respect, the ques- tions can be viewed as layers of intersectional inquiry.

These conceptual questions have implications for each stage of the research process (see Table 1). When research- ers ask who is included within a category, it encourages

them to understand all their participants in terms of the multiple social categories of identity, difference, and dis- advantage they represent and to attend to groups that are often overlooked in psychology. This question does not imply that any given study ought to include individuals representing every permutation of race, gender, class, or other social identity; not only is this practically impossible, it is properly the cooperative work of a field. Rather, attention to who is included within any category of interest, with particular attention to groups that have often been excluded, is meant to encourage psychologists to view all samples in terms of their particularity and to attend to diversity within samples. For example, a random sample of Black college students includes men and women, but it may not include proportional representation of youths from low-income families. Analyses should attend to gender differences within this sample, and the findings should not be interpreted to apply to Black youths in general. Psychol- ogists who ask this question may also be more likely to consider studying groups that have been overlooked by researchers. Reading the literature in psychology with this question in mind can make systematic omissions in sam- pling obvious.

This question also entails scrutiny of the manipula- tions and measures used to operationalize constructs: Which groups’ experiences do they reflect and represent? What samples were used in the development of the scales, and how might the instruments differ if other groups were included? For example, in her study of Black women’s race and gender identities, Settles (2006) included items about the extent to which these identities were experienced as conflicting; such questions would be less relevant—or even puzzling—for some other race/gender groups. Thus, reflecting on the implicit inclusions and exclusions in re- search can lead to greater cognizance of how the concep- tualization of social categories affects methodological de- cisions.

The question of what role inequality plays makes the greatest demands at the level of hypothesis generation and interpretation of findings. This question helps researchers view the participants and phenomena they study as grounded in social and historical contexts: Race, gender, sexuality, and class, as well as other social categories, structure groups’ access to social, economic, and political resources and privileges. Jackson and Williams’s (2006) work on public health crises among the Black middle class illustrates the insights resulting from this question. They noted that although higher social class is related to de- creased rates of suicide for Whites, the association is pos- itive for Black American men. To understand this finding, they pointed to three sources of psychological stress related to this group’s structural position in terms of race, class, and gender: stressors of racist experiences, the recency and fragility of middle-class status for many Blacks, and dis- appointment that occupational advancement has not been commensurate with educational achievement for many Black men. By conceptualizing race, gender, sexuality, and class as simultaneously shaping this group’s experience, Jackson and Williams looked for explanations in terms of

176 April 2009 ● American Psychologist

structural inequality upstream, rather than primarily at the level of individual differences. Of course, individual dif- ferences are also important for understanding suicide; how- ever, this second question draws psychologists’ attention to the ways categories of identity, difference, and disadvan- tage are also associated with individuals’ life chances and choices.

Asking what role inequality plays may lead research- ers to look for both similarities and differences across groups. This leads to the third question, Where are the similarities? This question represents the greatest departure from viewing social categories as defining fundamentally different types of people. Often researchers use social categories of identity, difference, and disadvantage primar- ily to define groups whose difference is a testable hypoth- esis, which, if not supported, defaults to similarity. Testing these differences rarely provides insight into the psycho- logical experience implicit in the categories or the practices that create and maintain them. If psychologists conceptu- alize social categories as defining structural relations with implications for individual, social, and institutional prac- tices, they must attend to both differences and similarities, even among groups that appear to be disparate. Because these similarities may not be obvious, addressing the ques- tion of commonalities across difference may entail con- ducting exploratory analyses or using interpretive qualita- tive methods. At the level of sampling, this question encourages researchers to include diverse groups within their studies, groups chosen not only in terms of group membership, but also in terms of shared relations to power. For example, qualitative research suggests that some work- ing-class White men feel that economic restructuring, changes in gender roles, and increased immigration have eroded privileges they previously held with respect to their status as earners, their gender, and their race (Weis, Prow- eller, & Centrie, 1997); this psychological sense of “being under siege” (p. 212) on multiple fronts might have some similarities to the experiences of Black middle-class men in terms of mental health outcomes.

What I am suggesting here is distinct from Hyde’s (2005) gender similarities hypothesis. Hyde argued that meta-analytic review of the gender difference literature finds many more similarities between women and men than differences; much of what might appear to be gender dif- ferences can be shown to be a function of the different contexts that men and women typically find themselves in by virtue of their social roles. In contrast, looking for commonality across differences does not suggest research- ers should reexamine the magnitude or extent to which there are differences between groups defined on one social category (e.g., gender). It is critically important from an intersectional standpoint that in recognizing similarities, researchers remain sensitive to nuanced differences across groups, even when similarities are found. For example, although middle-class Black men and working-class White men might experience some of the stressors they face in similar ways, their experiences are not equivalent or iden- tical.

What then are the implications of an intersectional analysis for research methods? Certainly the first tool that many research psychologists would reach for to address questions of how outcomes are related to multiple group memberships is a research design in which social catego- ries are treated as independent variables with main effects and interactions. Sociologist McCall (2005) termed this the “categorical approach” to intersectionality, which “fo- cus[es] on the complexity of relationships among multiple social groups within and across analytical categories. . . . The subject is multigroup and the method is systematically comparative” (p. 1786). This raises the question of whether the theoretical concept of intersectionality is equivalent to the inclusion of variables assessing race, gender (or other social categories), and their interaction in statistical models.

Certainly, this statistical method is an indispensable tool, particularly useful for revealing patterns of disparity in arenas such as employment and income, physical and mental health, and social life. Smith and Stewart (1983) described some of the patterns of group differences these interactions might describe: In some cases, the negative effects of racism and sexism might multiply each other, rendering women of color most disadvantaged on a depen- dent variable (e.g., income); in other cases, they might have “subtractive effects” (p. 7), in effect canceling each other out (Sanchez-Hucles, 1997). The “intersectionality para- dox” (Jackson & Williams, 2006, p. 138) in the minority health literature illustrates what can be gained by using such an approach. Although higher socioeconomic status is generally associated with better health outcomes, on many health indices, highly educated Blacks fare no better than Whites with the lowest education. Jackson and Williams (2006) identified this “largely unrecognized and high risk pattern” (p. 137) in two gendered health risks: infant mor- tality and homicide. In these examples, the interactive effects of race and gender suggest that even with the growth of the Black middle class since the civil rights movement, many middle-class Blacks do not enjoy the same outcomes as middle-class Whites.

Despite the power of this method to address certain intersectional research questions, it would be a mistake to reduce the nuanced theoretical concept of intersectionality to include only the type of associations that can be modeled through the use of interaction effects. One limitation to this approach arises from the fact that social categories, such as race and gender, are confounded in individuals; this means that any survey question that asks participants to report whether their experiences were a function of one category membership rather than another may be eliciting flawed data. A good example of this problem is racialized sexual harassment reported by women of color, including verbal harassment or sexual attention based on racial stereotypes or physical features believed to be racially distinctive (Buchanan & Ormerod, 2002; Cortina, 2001). To ask re- spondents to rate the extent to which such an experience was separately determined by gender and by race/ethnicity reflects an invalid conceptualization of how membership in social categories is experienced, essentially asking respon- dents to fit their experiences onto a procrustean bed.

177April 2009 ● American Psychologist

A study showing that Black women’s ratings of per- sonal general discrimination were a function of their re- ports of race rather than gender discrimination illustrates this problem (Levin, Sinclair, Veniegas, & Taylor, 2002). The authors concluded that the combined effects of racism and sexism did not determine Black women’s perceptions of general discrimination. However, respondents were not instructed how to report experiences they perceived as having elements of both racism and sexism. Black women may view such experiences as one aspect of racism, be- cause Black men have also been subject to gendered racism (Collins, 2004). In this case, the use of interaction effects also assumes that the effect of multiple category member- ships is to influence the quantity of outcomes, rather than interacting to influence the outcome qualitatively: As Levin et al. (2002) concluded, “types of discrimination perceived by various ethnic and gender groups may be qualitatively different and may not necessarily ‘add up’ or ‘interact’ in a statistical quantitative sense” (p. 560).

Testing intersectional research questions by looking at interactions between categories can undertheorize the pro- cesses that create the categories represented as independent variables. Put another way, treating race and gender as independent variables suggests that these social categories are primarily properties of individuals rather than reflec- tions of macrolevel social practices linked to inequality (Weber & Parra-Medina, 2003). For example, sociologists have found meaningful differences between White and Black Americans’ experiences of being middle class. Pri- marily because of many years of redlining practices in mortgage lending, Black families with middle-class in- comes have less than half the net worth of White families with comparable incomes (Conley, 1999); because of on- going residential segregation, Black middle-class commu- nities are more likely to be geographically surrounded by poor communities and to have higher rates of crime (Pat- tillo-McCoy, 1999). Compared to middle-class Whites, middle-class Blacks are more likely to have a sibling in poverty (Pattillo-McCoy & Heflin, 1999), and thus the poor may seem less distant than they do to members of the White middle class. For these and other reasons, the psy- chological experience of middle-class status may differ by race in important ways. Of course, it is possible to address some of these discrepancies by controlling for covariates in statistical models. However, to the extent that the meaning of one independent variable (e.g., class) varies depending on the level of the other independent variable (e.g., race) is neither acknowledged nor measured—that is, whether a given status is experienced in different ways depending on the other statuses held—an important aspect of intersec- tionality remains unaddressed and invisible. These obser- vations suggest that the inclusion of statistical interactions among race, gender, and other social categories in multi- variate analyses is not, in and of itself, sufficient to develop what Smith and Stewart (1983) called a “truly interactive model of racism and sexism” (p. 6) without reconceptual- izing the ways researchers use race, gender, and other social categories.

These problems are not intrinsic to a research design (or statistical model) in which social categories are treated as independent variables; rather, they arise from the ways that statistical interactions between variables based on cat- egory membership are often interpreted in the literature. Rather than prescribing—or proscribing—any particular research or data analysis technique, the concept of inter- sectionality entails a conceptual shift in the way researchers understand social categories.

For example, experiments often examine two or more independent variables both in terms of main effects and interactions. Nevertheless, experimental methods are not antithetical to intersectional analysis. For example, Shih, Pittinsky, and Ambady (1999) looked at how the intersect- ing identities of Asian American women affected math performance, considering that this group is stereotyped positively in this domain by virtue of ethnicity and is stereotyped negatively because of gender. Participants whose femaleness was made salient performed worse, whereas the performance of those whose ethnicity was primed was enhanced. In a second study, the ethnicity effect was not replicated in a sample of Asian American women in Vancouver, where the stereotype of Asians as especially skilled at math is less prevalent. This article, which made only within-group comparisons, provides an intersectional analysis in at least two respects. Obviously, it looks at an underrepresented group defined by multiple social locations. More subtly, it situates the intersection of ethnic and gender identities in different contexts, both through the priming manipulation and the cross-cultural approach. In these ways, it transcends a simple categorical conceptualization of the intersection of race and gender. This example highlights the ways that an intersectional analysis hinges on the conceptualization of race, gender, and other social categories, rather than the use (or avoid- ance) of particular methods.

Conclusion The three questions I have posed suggest that an intersec- tional analysis requires a conceptual shift, even a paradigm shift, in the ways psychologists understand social catego- ries, such that they take seriously the cultural and political history of groups, as well as the ways these socially con- structed categories depend on one another for meaning and are jointly associated with outcomes. This shift is consis- tent with recent calls in psychology (Helms, Jernigan, & Mascher, 2005; Smedley & Smedley, 2005) and other social sciences (e.g., Hancock, 2005; McCall, 2005).

At the same time, the examples I have chosen dem- onstrate that many of psychology’s familiar tools can be pressed into service to address research questions through an intersectional lens and that this type of framework neither requires, privileges, nor excludes multigroup com- parisons. Clearly, this work is already being undertaken in some parts of the discipline, such as feminist psychology. However, the application of the three intersectional ques- tions I have outlined here does require that researchers rethink the relationship between their conceptualization of

178 April 2009 ● American Psychologist

social categories and their methodological choices, as de- scribed in Table 1.

The skeptical reader may ask what the critical lens of intersectionality can add to his or her research program, particularly if the work is not focused on members of subordinated groups. Although grounded in the lived ex- perience and critique of those at the convergence of mul- tiple stigmatized identities, the implications of the concept of intersectionality are more expansive. As Hancock (2007b) has argued, intersectionality does not simply de- scribe a content specialization addressing issues germane to specific populations. Rather, it also is a paradigm for theory and research offering new ways of understanding the com- plex causality that characterizes social phenomena. The examples in this article illustrate the ways this analytic framework can help psychologists to look for causes of human behavior both upstream and downstream, to notice and hypothesize about the multiple paths that may lead individuals to the same or similar outcomes, and to under- stand the ways that different social categories depend on each other for meaning and, thus, mutually construct one another and work together to shape outcomes.

Intersectionality makes plain that gender, race, class, and sexuality simultaneously affect the perceptions, expe- riences, and opportunities of everyone living in a society stratified along these dimensions. To understand any one of these dimensions, psychologists must address them in com- bination; intersectionality suggests that to focus on a single dimension in the service of parsimony is a kind of false economy. This insight invites us to approach the study of social categories with more complexity and suggests ways to bring more nuance and context to our research on the social categories that matter most in a stratified society.

REFERENCES

Anzaldua, G. (1987). Borderlands/la frontera: The new mestiza. San Francisco: Aunt Lute.

Asch, A. (1984). Personal reflections. American Psychologist, 39, 551– 552.

Beale, F. (1970). Double jeopardy: To be Black and female. In T. Cade (Ed.), The Black woman: An anthology (pp. 90–100). New York: Signet.

Betancourt, H., & López, S. (1993). The study of culture, ethnicity, and race in American psychology. American Psychologist, 48, 629–637.

Bonilla-Silva, E. (1997). Rethinking racism: Toward a structural interpre- tation. American Sociological Review, 62, 465–480.

Buchanan, N. T., & Ormerod, A. J. (2002). Racialized sexual harassment in the lives of African American women. Women & Therapy, 25, 107–124.

Cohen, C. J. (1997). Punks, bulldaggers and welfare queens: The radical potential of queer politics. GLQ: A Journal of Lesbian and Gay Studies, 3, 437–466.

Cole, E. R., & Zucker, A. N. (2007). Black and White women’s perspec- tives on femininity. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 13, 1–9.

Collins, P. H. (1990). Black feminist thought: Knowledge, consciousness, and the politics of empowerment. New York: Routledge.

Collins, P. H. (2004). Black sexual politics: African Americans, gender and the new racism. New York: Routledge.

Combahee River Collective. (1995). Combahee River Collective state- ment. In B. Guy-Sheftall (Ed.), Words of fire: An anthology of African American feminist thought (pp. 232–240). New York: New Press. (Original work published 1977)

Conley, D. (1999). Being Black, living in the red: Race, wealth, and social policy in America. Berkeley: University of California Press.

Cortina, L. M. (2001). Assessing sexual harassment among Latinas: Development of an instrument. Cultural Diversity & Ethnic Minority Psychology, 7, 164–181.

Craig, M. L. (2002). Ain’t I a beauty queen? Black women, beauty and the politics of race. New York: Oxford University Press.

Crenshaw, K. (1993). Demarginalizing the intersection of race and sex: A Black feminist critique of antidiscrimination doctrine, feminist theory and antiracist politics. In D. K. Weisbert (Ed.), Feminist legal theory: Foundations (pp. 383–395). Philadelphia: Temple University Press. (Original work published 1989)

Crenshaw, K. W. (1994). Mapping the margins: Intersectionality, identity politics, and violence against women of color. In M. A. Fineman & R. Mykitiuk (Eds.), The public nature of private violence (pp. 93–118). New York: Routledge.

Davis, A. Y. (1983). Women, race and class. New York: Vintage Books. Dworkin, S. L. (2005). Who is epidemiologically fathomable in the

HIV/AIDS epidemic? Gender, sexuality and intersectionality in public health. Culture, Health & Sexuality, 7, 615–623.

Eagly, A., & Wood, W. (1999). The origins of sex differences in human behavior: Evolved dispositions versus social roles. American Psychol- ogist, 54, 408–423.

Espiritu, Y. L. (2001). “We don’t sleep around like the White girls do”: Family, culture, and gender in Filipina American lives. Signs, 26, 415–440.

Farough, S. D. (2006). Believing is seeing: The matrix of vision and White masculinities. Journal of Contemporary Ethnography, 35, 51– 83.

Fine, M., & Burns, A. (2003). Class notes: Toward a critical psychology of class and schooling, Journal of Social Issues, 59, 841–860.

Fine, M., & Weis, L. (1998). Crime stories: A critical look through race, ethnicity, and gender. Qualitative Studies in Education, 11, 435–459.

Giddings, P. (1985). When and where I enter: The impact of Black women on race and sex in America. New York: Morrow.

Glick, P., Lameiras, M., Fiske, S., Eckes, T., Masser, B., Volpato, C., et al. (2004, May). Bad but bold: Ambivalent attitudes toward men predict gender inequality in 16 nations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 713–728.

Hancock, A. (2005). W.E.B. DuBois: Intellectual forefather of intersec- tionality? Souls, 7(3–4), 74–84.

Hancock, A. (2007a). Examining intersectionality as a research paradigm: Multiplication doesn’t equal quick addition, Perspectives on Politics, 5, 63–79.

Hancock, A. (2007b). Intersectionality as a normative and empirical paradigm. Politics & Gender, 3, 248–254.

Helms, J. E., Jernigan, M., & Mascher, J. (2005). The meaning of race in psychology and how to change it. American Psychologist, 60, 27–36.

Higginbotham, E. B. (1992). African-American women’s history and the metalanguage of race. Signs, 17, 251–274.

hooks, b. (1984). Feminist theory: From margin to center. Boston: South End Press.

Hull, G. T., Scott, P. B., & Smith, B. (Eds.). (1982). All the women are White, all the Blacks are men, but some of us are brave. Old Westbury, NY: Feminist Press.

Hurtado, A. (1989). Relating to privilege: Seduction and rejection in the subordination of White women and women of color. Signs, 14, 833– 855.

Hyde, J. S. (2005). The gender similarities hypothesis. American Psychol- ogist, 60, 581–592.

Jackson, P. B., & Williams, D. (2006). The intersection of race, gender, and SES: Health paradoxes. In A. Schulz & L. Mullings (Eds.), Gender, race, class, and health: Intersectional approaches (pp. 131–162). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Jost, J. T., & Kruglanski, A. W. (2002). The estrangement of social constructionism and experimental social psychology: History of the rift and prospects for reconciliation. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 6, 168–187.

King, D. K. (1988). Multiple jeopardy, multiple consciousnesses: The context of a Black feminist ideology. Signs, 14, 42–72.

Knapp, G. (2005). Race, class, gender: Reclaiming baggage in fast trav- eling theories. European Journal of Women’s Studies, 12, 249–265.

179April 2009 ● American Psychologist

Kuriloff, P., & Reichert, M. (2003). Boys of class, boys of color: Nego- tiating the academic and social geography of an elite independent school. Journal of Social Issues, 59, 751–770.

Lau, A., Lum, S., Chronister, K. M., & Forrest, L. (2006). Asian American college women’s body image: A pilot study. Cultural and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 12, 259–274.

Levin, S., Sinclair, S., Veniegas, R., & Taylor, P. (2002). Perceived discrimination in the context of multiple social identities. Psychological Science, 13, 557–560.

Lott, B. (2002). Cognitive and behavioral distancing from the poor. American Psychologist, 57, 100–110.

Mahalingam, R. (2007). Culture, power and psychology of marginality. In A. Fuligni (Ed.), Contesting stereotypes and creating identities: Social categories, social identities, and educational participation (pp. 42–65). New York: Sage.

Mahalingam, R., & Leu, J. (2005). Culture, essentialism, immigration and representations of gender. Theory & Psychology, 15, 839–860.

McCall, L. (2005). The complexity of intersectionality. Signs, 30, 1771– 1800.

Pattillo-McCoy, M. (1999). Black picket fences: Privilege and peril among the Black middle class. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Pattillo-McCoy, M., & Heflin, C. M. (1999, August). Poverty in the family: Siblings of the Black and White middle classes. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Sociological Association, Chi- cago.

Purdie-Vaughns, V., & Eibach, R. P. (2008). Intersectional invisibility: The distinctive advantages and disadvantages of multiple subordinate- group identities. Sex Roles, 59, 377–391.

Reid, P. T. (1993). Poor women in psychological research: Shut up and shut out. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 17, 133–150.

Risman, B. J. (2004). Gender as a social structure: Theory wrestling with activism. Gender & Society, 18, 429–450.

Sanchez-Hucles, J. V. (1997). Jeopardy not bonus status for African American women in the workforce: Why does the myth of advantage persist? American Journal of Community Psychology, 25, 565–580.

Savin-Williams, R., & Diamond, L. (2000). Sexual identity trajectories

among sexual-minority youths: Gender comparisons. Archives of Sex- ual Behavior, 29, 607–627.

Schooler, D., Ward, L. M., Merriwether, A., & Caruthers, A. (2004). Who’s that girl: Television’s role in the body image development of young White and Black women. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 28, 38–47.

Settles, I. H. (2006). Use of an intersectional framework to understand Black women’s racial and gender identities. Sex Roles, 54, 589–601.

Shih, M., Pittinsky, T., & Ambady, N. (1999). Stereotype susceptibility: Identity salience and shifts in quantitative performance. Psychological Science, 10, 80–83.

Silverstein, L. B. (2006). Integrating feminism and multiculturalism: Scientific fact or science fiction? Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 37, 21–28.

Smedley, A., & Smedley, B. (2005). Race as biology is fiction, racism as a social problem is real: Anthropological and historical perspectives on the social construction of race. American Psychologist, 60, 16–26.

Smith, A., & Stewart, A. J. (1983). Approaches to studying racism and sexism in Black women’s lives. Journal of Social Issues, 39(3), 1–15.

Spelman, E. V. (1988). Inessential woman. Boston: Beacon. Stewart, A. J., & McDermott, C. (2004). Gender in psychology. Annual

Review of Psychology, 55, 519–544. Sue, D. W. (2004). Whiteness and ethnocentric monoculturalism: Making

the “invisible” visible. American Psychologist, 59, 761–769. Sue, S. (1999). Science, ethnicity, and bias: Where have we gone wrong?

American Psychologist, 54, 1070–1077. Weber, L., & Parra-Medina, D. (2003). Intersectionality and women’s

health: Charting a path to eliminating health disparities. In V. Demos & M. T. Segal (Eds.), Advances in gender research: Gender perspectives on health and medicine (pp. 181–230). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

Weis, L., Proweller, A., & Centrie, C. (1997). Re-examining “a moment in history”: Loss of privilege inside White working-class masculinity in the 1990s. In M. Fine, L. Weis, L. C. Powell, & L. M. Wong (Eds.), Off-White: Readings on race, power and society (pp. 210–226). Lon- don: Routledge.

180 April 2009 ● American Psychologist

Get a 10 % discount on an order above $ 50
Use the following coupon code :
TOPCLASS